Lecture 3
Approaches to Research
Qualitative and Quantitative methods
Dr Lincoln Colling
10 Oct 2022
Psychology as a Science
Approaches to research
Doing research is an integral part of your training as a psychologist.
But before you can start thinking about doing research you need to be aware of the different approaches that are available to you.
In today’s lecture, we’ll cover approaches from the two traditional divisions:
qualitative methods and
quantitative methods
And we’ll finish off by talking about how computer simulation can be used in psychology research
Qualitative and Quantitative methods
We can split approaches to research into two broad categories
We can give some simple descriptions of these categories:
Quantitative methods collect numbers/numerical data and use statistical tools
Qualitative methods collect words, pictures, and artefacts
Some researchers also adopt both approaches (mixed-methods) or apply quantitative methods to qualitative style data.
Quantitative methods are probably easier to group together, because many approaches can be grouped under qualitative methods.
This course focuses on quantitative methods, but you’ll learn more about qualitative methods in later years.
Outline of quantitative methods
Quantitative approaches take a phenomenon and try to condense it down into a few dimensions or variables that can be measured as precisely and reliably as possible.
It is very important to choose variables that are representative of the phenomenon you’re studying.
Choosing variables that are representative of the phenomenon you’re interested in involves operationalisation.
Operationalisation means choosing a measurable proxy for the phenomenon you’re interested in1
Outline of quantitative methods
Quantitative approaches often make use of statistical methods
Using statistical methods means looking at lots of cases (for example, studying lots of people, not just one or two).
The goal with quantitative methods is often to develop generalisations, or theories that are generally applicable.
Involves testing predictions that logically follow from theories (the deductive step)1
Outline of qualitative methods
Qualitative methods are focused on meaning rather than measurement.
Instead of condensing a phenomenon down to a simple set of features or dimensions, qualitative research tries to examine many features.
Qualitative approaches try to look at all aspects of one or a few instances of a phenomenon.
Qualitative approaches view the context (physical environment, social setting, cultural context) as a central part of the phenomenon being studied.
Qualitative approaches—e.g., grounded theory and phenomenology—also emphasise the idea of following the data wherever it leads (that is, the inductive step)1
Qualitative methods
Qualitative methods are extremely varied with different methodologies, underlying theoretical assumptions, and intellectual histories
I can’t do them all justice in one short lecture, so we’ll only cover a few:
- Verbal protocol analysis
- Ethnographic methods
- Discourse analysis
- Phenomenology
However, there are many more, including Case Studies, Grounded Theory, Participatory Research, Focus Groups…
I’ll try to draw out some contrasts between qualitative and quantitative methods more generally and highlight strengths and weaknesses of each approach.
Verbal protocol analysis
Also known as “thinking aloud protocols” (or “talking aloud protocols”)
Involves collecting and analysing verbal data on cognitive processing
Participants are given a task (usually a task that involves multiple steps that are chained together) and are asked to verbalise (speak aloud) what they are thinking as they go about solving the task
Data (i.e., recordings of what the participant said) are coded and analysed to infer the information processing steps involved in solving the problem
The approach was used in early Cognitive Science by Simon and Newell who were pioneering researchers in Cognitive Science and Artificial Intelligence (Computational Theory of Mind)
Carries certain assumptions about the nature of human cognition/thinking, e.g., that it involves information processing in discrete sequential steps.
Example Verbal Protocol Analysis
1 |
OK… the first thing I would do is to make a list of the shops that are quite close to each other |
Executive |
Generate plan |
2 |
and highlight the dance class remembering that it is at a specific time |
Metaplan |
Satisfy time constraints |
3 |
I would try to get to it first and get it over with… |
Executive |
Order messages |
4 |
probably, in reality I would drop it…. |
Executive |
Evaluate plan Eliminate |
Ethnography
More a style of research than a method of data collection, ethnography involves studying people in “the field” (i.e., their naturally occurring setting), and requires the researcher to enter into the setting they are studying
Attempts to understand how the socio-cultural practices and behaviours of people are shaped by their social, physical, and cultural contexts
Tries to make sense of events from the perspective of the participants
Could include data from interviews, or participant observation1
Ethnography
In cognitive psychology, ethnographic approaches have been used to understand how people solve problems in real-world settings.
For example, how do technological artefacts (that is, the context) support cognitive processing:
One famous study in cognitive ethnography involves studying how sailors use the technological artefacts (instruments etc) and layout of a ship to help them navigate
In critical psychology, ethnographic approaches have been used to understand the interplay between, race, class, gender, and education in shaping participants’ life worlds
Discourse analysis
Discourse analysis is the social study of language as used in talk, text, and other forms of communication.
It involves a distinctive way of thinking about talk and text where language doesn’t just represent the world but also constructs the world.
Some questions one might examine with this approach:
How does language shape social relations? For example, how might certain kinds of talk establish professional distance in doctor-patient communication?
How might language construct or open up space for particular identities. For example, how might language enforce or break down the concept of binary gender?
Discourse analysis
The strengths of this approach are that it allows you to examine how language constructs reality.
It can make use of primary data (interviews, talk in focus groups) or secondary data (books, newspaper articles).
But it can be difficult to use discourse analysis to develop the same kind of generalisations as you might develop with other approaches.
Phenomenology
Particularly associated with the philosophers Husserl, Merleau-Ponty, and Sartre
The phenomenological approach involves bracketing off any preconceived notions we might have about a phenomenon to achieve an understanding of that phenomenon that has not been influenced by our prior beliefs.
Emphasises peoples first-hand experience and attempts to understand and describe subjective experience from the participant’s point of view
Used in fields like cognitive psychology to understand the nature of subjective sensory experiences
A phenomenological approach to studying, for example, inclusive classroom settings might try to understand what it is like for a student with a disability to be in that classroom setting.
Contrast with an ethnographic approach that might look at how the classroom setting changes interactions between students with and without disabilities
Issues in qualitative research
Unlike quantitative methods than might use printed questionnaires or computers to record and measure responses, in qualitative research, the researcher is the instrument
important for researchers to reflect on their values, assumptions, biases, and beliefs to understand how these might impact the research
the research instrument (i.e., the researcher) can change. For example, in ethnographic research, the changes in the researchers experience might alter how they record and observe behaviours.
There are parallels to validity (internal and external), reliability, and “objectivity” in qualitative research1
These are Credibility, Transferability, Dependability, and Confirmability
Issues in qualitative research
Credibility: Can the data support the claims. Can be established through prolonged engagement, discussions with other researchers/participants, and critical self-reflection
Transferability: Can the findings be transferred to similar contexts. Requires extensive, detailed, and careful descriptions of the research context (“thick descriptions”).
Dependability: Ensuring that researchers maintain a record of changes in the research process or research instrument (i.e., themselves) over time.
Confirmability: Concerned with ensuring that the data used to support the conclusions are verifiable.
Quantitative methods
As the name suggests, a key aspect of quantitative methods is quantification.
Quantification means putting numbers to the thing we’re interested in studying so that it can be measured.
The motivation behind measuring phenomena is that measurements are publicly available and verifiable (e.g., scientists can check or verify your measurements).
Unlike qualitative research where researchers try to simultaneously study many aspects of a single phenomenon, quantitative research tries to condense a phenomenon down into a single (or a few) dimension(s).
The first step in quantitative research is often figuring out how to quantify the phenomenon of interest. This involves choosing a proxy (something measurable) that can stand-in for the phenomenon
Operationalisation
If you’re interested in anxiety, you have to decide how to measure anxiety. You can’t measure an abstract concept directly.
The process of choosing a proxy is known as operationalisation.
There are lots of ways to choose a measurable proxy that can stand in for anxiety.
Two ways might be to:
Develop a scale or a questionnaire.
Measure physiological responses like increased heart rate or galvanic skin response.
Operationalisation
Measurements have to be reliable (reproducible) and valid (actually measure what you think you’re measuring).
For example, if we develop a scale for depression then the scale must produce similar numbers when applied to:
And a treatment for depression, should not just reduce scores on our depression scale, but it must also result in people experiencing less depression.
Quantitative methods and causation
Qualitative research studies phenomena in the wild
But Quantitative approaches instead try to exert a lot of control over phenomena.
Control allows researchers to make claims about causation and give causal explanations
There are a few ways to understand causation:
What is a cause?
One view of causation can be summed up as a difference that makes a difference:
If you take two situations, one in which the phenomenon occurs and another in which does not occur then whatever is different between those situations is the cause of the phenomenon.
For example, take one situation in which a window is broken and another in which a window isn’t broken:
If the only difference between the two is:
Then a boy throwing a rock is the cause of the broken window.
What is a cause?
You can also understand causation in terms of manipulation:
If you can manipulate one thing and observe a change in another, then the two things may be causally connected.
As I put my foot down or lift it from the accelerator pedal I can observe a change in the speed of the car
So I know the accelerator pedal and the speed of the car are causally connected
By intervening and manipulating parts of a system you can identify how they work (you can identify mechanisms).
What is a cause?
Causation can also be understood in terms of probability:
If the presence of one thing increases the probability of the other thing occurring, then there may be a causal relationship.
The presence of smoking increases the probability of developing cancer, so smoking may be the cause of cancer.
Causation and confounds
In the examples above they are all examples of possible causes
To be justified in claiming a causal relationship other conditions must usually be met
But causal claims are not always black and white. Sometimes we can only be more or less sure about causal relationships.
What are some of the other conditions that need to be met?
An example of Smoking and Cancer
The presence of smoking increases the probability of developing cancer, so smoking may be the cause of cancer.
Having emphysema also increases the probability of developing cancer. But is emphysema the cause of cancer?
There is a plausible mechanism of action between smoking and cancer but not between emphysema and cancer, so we can be more sure that smoking causes cancer than we can be about emphysema causing cancer.
A more likely explanation is that emphysema and cancer have a common cause—smoking.
Smoking and confounds
Let’s say you are studying the relationship between emphysema and cancer, because you think emphysema might cause cancer
In this situation, smoking is a confound
If you wanted to see whether emphysema caused cancer then you’d have to control for smoking
Only look at smokers and see if there’s still a relationship between emphysema and cancer or whether cancer also occurs in the absence of emphysema
Only look at non-smokers and see whether emphysema and cancer are still related or whether cancer develops in the absence of emphysema
Emphysema and cancer are correlated (the increase in one leads to an increase in the other), but emphysema doesn’t cause cancer because they have a common cause.
Correlation and causation
Sometimes two correlated variables have a causal relationship: smoking and cancer
Sometimes they have a common cause: emphysema and cancer
And sometimes they have neither:
From 2000 to 2009, there was a strong relationship between the number of men getting engineering degrees and per capita consumption of sour cream.
An example of a spurious correlation
Qualitative vs Quantitative methods
In qualitative research, you study phenomena in context while in quantitative research you aim for control.
But you can use either approach to study the same phenomena/psychological processes. Let’s say you’re interested in memory:
How could you study memory from:
Quantitative study of memory
You could use experiments in a lab where you give people lists of words to remember. You could manipulate aspects of the words—for example, their emotional salience—and measure performance (accuracy scores) to try to understand something about memory and emotional salience.
Ensure that the only thing that differs between the words on each list is the emotional salience.
Control for possible confounds like:
Word length: make sure that one list doesn’t contain long words and the other short words
Word order: make sure some people get the lists in one order and some in the other order, because maybe people get tired by the end and that influences memory.
Qualitative study of memory
For a qualitative approach you don’t want to study memory in the lab—you want to study it in the wild. This allows you to ask different kinds of questions.
You could use an ethnographic approach with, for example, bartenders:
You might do fieldwork in a bar observing bartenders.
Through this, you might see that bartenders structure their environment in a particular way—e.g., put certain types of glasses or bottles in particular places.
This might lead you to form the hypothesis that bartenders structure their environment to support their memory—i.e., placing certain bottles and glasses together helps them remember what goes in what kinds of cocktails.
Computer simulation
Computer simulation has been used to study a lot of different phenomena in psychology, but here are some examples of approaches I find particularly interesting.
Simulation has been used to show how seemingly complex behaviour can arise from very simple processes.
Flocking behaviour in birds seems very complex, and it looks as if there must be something very complex going on inside their brains.
But you can simulate this behaviour with only three simple rules:
- avoid collisions with other birds
- align direction with nearby birds
- approach distant birds
Example simulation of flocking birds
In the lecture notes you can adjust the rules of the simulation
Conway’s game of life1
Conway’s Game Of Life has 4 simple rules:
A live cell with 2 or 3 live neighbours lives on
A live cell with < 2 live neighbours dies (underpopulation)
A live cell with > 3 live neighbours dies (overpopulation)
A dead cell with exactly 3 live neighbours becomes a live cell (reproduction)
Agent-based modelling (ABM)
Agent-based modelling takes a cue from approaches like those used to model bird flocking and Conway’s Game of Life.
In an agent-based model, the researcher simulates a group of ‘agents’.
The ‘agents’ will typically have some memory, a set of goals, and some rules.
The memory allows them to store their current state or consequences of their previous actions.
The goals usually represent some state they’re trying to achieve.
And rules govern their interactions.
By allowing these agents to interact, and by manipulating aspects of the agents (their memory, goals, and rules) it’s possible to see how social phenomena can arise.
Agent-based modelling (ABM)
Agent-based modelling can be used for modelling phenomena like the spread of misinformation through social groups:
If you thought that misinformation was more likely to spread if passed on by particularly influential individuals (e.g., celebrities or politicians), then you could include this idea in your simulation.
Or if you thought that misinformation was more likely to spread inside socially isolated groups, then you could modify your simulation to create socially isolated groups to test this hypothesis.
After your simulation, you could still go and check the real world to see if it behaves like your simulation.
Final thoughts
There are a lot of different methodological approaches available to you in psychology, and this lecture has just touched on a few
You won’t cover computer simulation again in undergrad, but if you stay on to do a Masters degree (or a third year project with me) then you might get the chance
Next year, you’ll learn more about qualitative methods…
… But for the rest of this course we’ll dive more deeply into quantitative methods